Developmental topographagnosia

We continually rely on our abilities of spatial navigation, be it for the daily commute to work, a trip to the local supermarket, or simply to make our way to the bathroom in the middle of the night. These tasks involve complex cognitive processes, yet most people perform them effortlessly and some develop them to a remarkable degree. Take, for example, London taxi drivers, who have a highly detailed knowledge of the 25,000 streets that lie within a six-mile radius of Charing Cross station, as well as the locations of thousands of "points", or landmarks, such as nightclubs, hospitals, hotels, theatres, railway and police stations, places of worship, parks and government, historical and public buildings.

Because most of us make little or no conscious effort to navigate our environment, we find it difficult to imagine being unable to do so. But there are numerous reports of people who have impaired spatial navigation. Such deficits are associated with damage to the various parts of the brain involved in attention, perception or memory, which can occur as a result of traumatic brain injury, epilepsy or Alzheimer's Disease. Now researchers from the Brain Research Center at the University of British Columbia report the case of a woman who has never been able to orient in her environment, even in the absence of any apparent brain damage. The study, published in the journal Neuropsychologia, is the first documented case of what the researchers have named developmental topographagnosia (or topographical disorientation).

When one navigates through a novel environment, the brain generates cognitive maps of the new surroundings. The idea of a cognitive map dates back to a seminal 1948 paper by the psychologist Edward Tolman. Based on his experiments in which rats were taught their way through a maze, Tolman concluded that "in the course of learning something like a field map of the environment gets established in the rat's brain," and that "incoming impulses are usually worked over and elaborated...into a tentative, cognitive-like map of the environment... indicating routes and paths and environmental relationships."

We now know that Tolman's cognitive maps are generated by the hippocampus and surrounding temporal lobe structures, which are crucial for the spatial memory which is required for navigation. Lesions in the hippocampal formation often lead to a disorder known as anterograde disorientation, in which the ability to learn new routes is impaired. But navigation involves more than just spatial memory, and engages a diffuse network of regions distributed throughout the brain. For example, patients with lesions in the retrosplenial cortex near the boundary between the parietal and occipital lobes are unable to obtain directional information from landmarks; those with damage to the fusiform gyrus, found at the junction between the occipital and temporal lobes, are unable to recognize the landmarks themselves; and those with damage to regions toward the back of the parietal cortex cannot determine the position of landmarks relative to themselves. 

The new case study is of a 43-year-old left-handed woman known as Patient 1 (Pt1). According to her parents, her language and cognitive development were normal but has never been able to navigate. From about the age of 6, she recalls panicking at the grocery store whenever her mother disappeasred from sight. She completed high school successfully, but for the entire 12 years of her schooling had to be taken to and from school by her sisters or parents. Each time she tried to leave home by herslef, she got lost, and so relied on friends to accompany her wherever she went. 

The woman now lives with her father and is employed as a public servant. She has no difficulty discriminating left from right and easily recognizes familiar places and landmarks. But she follows a streotyped route to get to the office where she has worked for 5 years - every day, she takes the same bus dowtown and knows where to get off because she recognizes a distinctive square; from there, she follows a straight 30 meter route to her office block. To get home, she follows exactly the same route in the reverse direction, but gets lost with even the smallest deviation from that route; she also often gets lost in her neighbourhood and has to call her father to ask him to get her.

After being told that her office would be relocating, Pt1 sought help and was eventually referred to neuropsychologist Giuseppe Iaria and his colleagues at the University of Columbia's Human Vision and Eye Movement Laboratory. Here, she took part in a series of detailed investigations, including functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), neuropsychological evaluations designed to test general intelligence, memory and visuospatial abilities and various assessments of her navigational skills. Her performance on these tests was compared to that of a control group consisting of four left-handed females.

Pt1 was found to be fluent and to have normal verbal understanding. Her short- and long-term memory were also found to be normal; she showed no signs of deficits in attention or perception and scored just below average on an IQ test. Neuroimaging did not reveal any signs of haemorrhage or ischemia, and no structural abnormalities were detected in the hippocampal formation or any other part of the brain.   

To further examine Pt1's navigational skills, Iaria and his colleagues administered a series of real-world tests performed in a part of the city that was unfamiliar to her, which aimed to assess the strategies she uses to orient herself. In one of these tests, she was asked to follow a researcher from one place to a target location and was then led back to the starting point via a different route. She was then asked to return along the original route to the target location. In a similar test, the researcher indicated during the initial phase various landmarks located at crossings.

Pt1 performed both of these tests accurately. During the test phase on the first one, she named the buildings she was looking at whilst navigating and later confirmed that she was using the buildings and landmarks rather than referring to the signs showing the names of the streets. Thus she was able to learn a path which she had previously travelled and to navigate it successfully. The patient also had no difficulty in navigating a route using a list of instructions given to her by the researchers. developmental_topographic_disorientation1.jpg

When given a map to navigate, however, she ran into problems. One of the map-based navigation tasks, she did not take the shortest possible route to the specified destination, and made a wrong turning on the way. When asked to draw a  floor plan of the house in which she lived, Pt1 produced a map (above left) containing the correct number of rooms in their proper sequence. However, although she could tell the researchers where various objects could be found in the house, she was unable to locate any of them on her plan. When her map was compared to one produced by her father (above right), it became clear that Pt1's map was greatly distorted and had an inaccurate scale.

Similar results were obtained when she was asked to draw a map of her route to work. However, when she was asked to produce maps of unfamiliar places, such as the main routes of the city or her neighbourhood, she was unable to, and stated that she did not have "in my mind a map to report". Thus, it appears that although Pt1 can create approximate representations of environments with which she is very familiar, based on the procedures she had learnt to navigate them, she has a selective difficulty in forming spatial representations of less familiar environments.

To test this possibility, the researchers administered several tests which assessed Pt1's ability to form and use cognitive maps in a virtual city. Created using the editor of a three-dimensional gaming software, the city consisted of several buildings of different shapes and sizes and four clearly identifiable landmarks (a cinema, a restaurant, a hotel and a flower shop). In one task, the patient was asked to navigate freely through the virtual city and to learn the locations of the landmarks, and their spatial relationships, as she did so. In the second, she was required to draw upon her mental representation of the virtual city to find the shortest possible route to reach the location of one of the landmarks.

Pt1 performed significantly worse on these tests than did the controls. It took her more than 30 minutes to form a cognitive map of the virtual environment, compared to an average of 11 minutes for the controls. Her exploration of the environment invovled covering the same route over and over again and she later reported that instead of devising a specific strategy she was trying to remember the locations of the landmarks with respect to one another. She said that she had difficulty remembering where the landmarks were in relation to her location; each time she encountered another landmark, she got lost. Likewise, Pt1 took significantly longer than the controls to find specific landmarks. On average, it took her 75 seconds to do so, compared to an average of around 16 seconds for the controls. However, it was found that she could learn and use a cognitive map of a simplified environment after intensive training.

Finally, the researchers used fMRI to assess the pattern of activation in Pt1's brain as she navigated through the same virtual city. Her results were compared to those of 9 healthy controls who had performed exactly the same tasks during a previous study. As Pt1 formed a map of the virtual city, significant increases in neural activity were observed in distributed regions of the frontal, temporal and parietal lobes. Exactly the same pattern was found in the healthy controls. However, unlike the controls, the patient showed no increase in activity of the hippocampus or retrosplenial cortex, the two regions which are known to be critical in cognitive map formation.

There is an important difference between this case and those of patients with navigation deficits as a result of brain damage. The latter often have other cognitive impairments, such as attention or memory deficits, which makes it difficult to determine exactly which process is being disrupted. Pt1, on the other hand, has a highly selective disorder which apparently results in an inability to form cognitive maps. Actually, she was not completely incapable of this task. One week after she first took the virtual city tests, Pt1 started a training program consisting of six one-hour sessions per week. Afterwards, the time it took her to form a map of the same virtual city was reduced to 5 minutes, and the time taken to find a specific landmark to just 4 seconds.

Surprisingly though, the patient appears not to have attempted to form such maps of her home or of her route to and from work. Rather, she seems to have adopted a life-long strategy which minimizes her need to form maps, because she has great difficulty doing so. This may provide clues to the origin of her disorder. In the now infamous studies of London taxi drivers, Eleanor Maguire and her colleagues showed that there is a strong correlation between the size of the hippocampus and the amount of experience in the job - the longer one has been driving a taxi, the larger is their hippocampus. It therefore seems plausible that a reluctance to form cognitive maps, starting from an early age and continuing throughout life, could result in a reduced capacity for the hippocampus to perform this particular function.

Iaria and his colleagues note that firm conclusions should not be drawn from neuroimaging studies involving just one participant. But they also suggest that the imaging data are useful, because small increases in hippocampal activity were detected following the intensive training sessions. The researchers believe that there are likely to be more people with developmental topographical disorientation, and have created a website called website in the hope of finding them and gaining a better understanding of the disorder.


ResearchBlogging.org

Iaria, G. et al (2008). Developmental topographical disorientation: Case one Neuropsychologia DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.021

More like this

Just trying to catch up with the publicartions in various PLoS Journals this past week. Here are some interesting titles. As always, you should rate the articles, post notes and comments and send trackbacks when you blog about the papers. You can now also easily place articles on various social…
Getting Lost: A Newly Discovered Developmental Brain Disorder: Feeling lost every time you leave your home? You may not be as alone as you think. Researchers at the University of British Columbia and Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute recently documented the first case of a patient who,…
Regular readers know an interest of mine are infections that cause more than just the typical acute spectrum of disease. For example, I've written on the role microbes might play in obesity, or the role of viruses in chronic disease such as cancer and, of course, AIDS. Still, typically,…
There are 57 new articles in PLoS ONE this week. As always, you should rate the articles, post notes and comments and send trackbacks when you blog about the papers. Here are my own picks for the week - you go and look for your own favourites: How Do Humans Control Physiological Strain during…

When I was a scout in my youth, I was one of the best map readers / navigators in our troop. I had an instinctive ability to navigate by dead reckoning and could easily interpret a map against my surroundings.

Several years ago I had a bad case of mono. For many months after having the disease I suffered from a lack of concentration and (sometimes severe) tiredness.

One day on university campus, a visitor came up to me and asked me to give her directions using a map of the campus that she handed me. I immediately said ok, confident that I could help, but found the map utterly confusing, I just stood there, staring at the map trying to makes sense of it. Most disconcerting.

When I visited new places, I also found that I could no longer rely on my ability to dead reckon, and had to take steps like remember landmarks or ask people for directions.

My ability to navigate came back after a while, but I don't think it's back to how it used to be.

Just thought I'd share.

By foolfodder (not verified) on 23 Sep 2008 #permalink

Mo, I found this post absolutely fascinating. I work with a professor who is trying to understand the areas involved in natural scene categorisation, in which we have used the PPA and RSC. I had no idea that RSC was involved in cognitive mapping, but now that I think about it, it makes sense, as it showed up as an area that seemed important in the natural scene categorisation study. Is the PPA used at all, or is it just the hippocampus?

I have another question - why do you call the study of taxicab drivers hippocampii (to coin a word) 'infamous'? It seems like fairly interesting work to me.

Very nice post.

Navaneethan:

RSC is heavily involved in navigation, and in the scene recognition literature is usually associated with locating where in world-based coordinates places are located (e.g, see Russell Epstein's recent work on it). Lesions there are known to significantly impair navigation.

Tim

Thanks for writing the article. I really appreciate that. It is important to let people know about this issue.

Please note that we called this disorder "developmental topographical disorientation" (not developmental topographagnosia - I am referring to the title), they are not exactly the same thing: topographagnosia is an "old" term meaning the inability to recognize the environment; whereas, topographical disorientation is a more general term includig not only the inability to recognize the environment but other disorders that all lead to disorientation. In so many cases people are able to recognize the environment but they do not know which directions to go, so topographical disorientation is more appropriate in these cases.

Regards,

Giuseppe Iaria

http://www.neurolab.ca/
http://www.gettinglost.ca/

I'm working on a painting right now called "Memory of Space." It's an impressionist view of the hippocampus. Reading you has inspired me, Mo.

By carolyn13 (not verified) on 25 Sep 2008 #permalink

Thanks for fixing my typo, Mo. It was bothering me. We'll see what happens with the painting. Maybe I'll make the breakthrough on it soon.

By carolyn13 (not verified) on 30 Sep 2008 #permalink

As a teenager of about fourteen years of age I was periodically hit on the head (with fist I don't remember)by my father who was right-handed.I don't remember which hemisphere of the brain but I suspect it was the right one. However I displayed topographgnosia even as a child of four. I feared opening opening my eyes as the surroundings appeared foreign and terrifying to my. I often closed my eyes to avoid the terror of what appeared to look foreign. I recall being unable to conceptualize even at that young age as well as being unable to navigate or memorize, particularly, stores. As an adult I cannot even picture my deceased mother, cannot remember the shape of my house, though I can recall the outlay of the interior. I cannot tell you what my neighbor's house looks like and have to be driven to to and from destination A to B over and over again. This is in contrast to superior language skills. What's wrong with me? I have no imagery. I cannot picture anything topological outside and cannot visualize anything in the "mind's eye." It has crippled me all my life.

Maxine Wilson

By Maxine Wilson (not verified) on 03 Aug 2009 #permalink

Addendum:

I forgot! I was always beaten on the back of my head. I suffer from bipolar disease and agraphobia. If you could write me back I would appreciate it, though I suspect I'd have to see a neurologist and have an MRI to determine the neurological aspects of what's is going on. I guess nothing could tell me the etiology though I don't have Alzheimers.

Your article was fascinating. By the way, I have difficulting comprehending reading. I have to re-read several times before I ascertain what is being said. I suppose that is related to my visual pathology. Ironically, I majored in English At UC Berkeley and received mostly A's in my subjects!

Thanks,

Maxine

Maxine

By Maxine Wilson (not verified) on 03 Aug 2009 #permalink

I'm not sure if I have EXACTLY this. But it's something that's fascinated me for years. I ALWAYS have a strong sense of direction, but it's not always right. I can be walking in a straight line toward a house, for example, facing South East. Go through the front door, make no turns, all of a sudden I visualize myself facing South West, for example. I know one of the directions has to be false, but my mind won't shake it.

I call this imagined direction my "Psychospace"... and usually that house will have that same Psychospace every time I visit it... it will always be wrong the same way.

This rarely bothers me... in fact I find it entertaining, and am perpetually fascinated by the many other ways my psychospace plays tricks on me. Roads only go from NW to SE, or SW to NE (Roads I'm traveling on, that is... I can visualize roads feeding into the road I'm on as facing N, S, E, or W... but if I TURN on that road, I "correct" it's direction).

Rooms always have N, S, E, and W at the corners. Needless to say, hexagonal rooms confuse my psychospace in delightful ways.

And abstract concepts have psychospace. Often based on real experience. The years, for the most part, run South East to North West (as it turns out, the first home I lived in as a kid was South East of the 2nd home, the 2nd home was South East of the Elementary school I was to later attend, and the first 3 grades followed the same sequence). However, things are different for the day, week, or month, based on the layouts of the calendar and clocks, and what wall of the house we placed them on when I was a kid!

I have Psychospaces for records, books, even TV and Radio channels... all based on where they were in the house I grew up in.

So, that's a thumbnail sketch of my experience with Psychospace. Hope you find this interesting!

I am in graduate school studying occupational therapy and I have trouble explaining concepts to others or participating in normal conversation. I also have a horible sence of direction. My wife has to tell me where to go. I have to go to a place many times before I know how to get there. I can only remember a couple turns at a time. Is my trouble in social communication related to this disorder. I have never been in a traumatic accident so what could be causing these problems.

By Jeremy Bramwell (not verified) on 26 Oct 2009 #permalink