Do insects feel pain?

Blogging on Peer-Reviewed Research

Using sophisticated techniques to silence or activate specific neurons, researchers from Stanford University have established that a simple behaviour used by fruit fly larvae to evade attack from parasitic wasps is triggered by a type of sensory neuron that is similar to the neurons which respond to painful stimuli in mammals.

Although little is known about the somatosensory system of fruit flies, several lines of evidence have implicated sensory cells called multidendritic neurons as nociceptors (cells that are responsive to noxious chemical, mechanical or thermal stimuli).

First, with their multiply branched and naked endings which innervate the epidermis, they resemble the nociceptors of vertebrates. Second, multidendritic neurons express an ion channel called painless, which is necessary for the response to noxious stimuli, and which was identified in a mutant fly strain that is unresponsive to such stimuli.

The researchers used genetic targeting to deliver tetanus toxin specifically to the four different types of multidendritic neurons. This effectively silences the cells by binding to and destroying one of the proteins involved in neurotransmitter release.

In larvae with type IV neurons silenced in this way, the rolling behaviour in response to noxious thermal stimuli applied by the researchers was abolished. By contrast, in larvae with types I and II neurons silenced, the rolling behaviour was present but slightly uncoordinated, suggesting that these cells are involved in proprioception.

The researchers then used a newly-developed technique called channelrhodopsin photoactivation to activate and inhibit the neurons rapidly. They found that pulses of blue light which activated the type IV neurons would also elicit the rolling behaviour, and that rolling ceased in response to pulses of a differently-coloured light that inactivated the cells. Light activation of type II and III neurons elicited in the larvae a distinct accordion-like wave of muscle contractions.

Surprisingly, the rolling tended to occur in the direction of, rather than away from, the heat probe used by the experimenters to elicit the behaviour. This was found to help the larvae evade attacks from parasitic wasps: rolling may deflect the wasp's oviposter, which normally penetrates the cuticle during an attack so that the wasp can lay an egg inside the larva.

Type IV neurons share another similarity with their mammalian counterparts: they are the only type of multidendritic neurons whose processes cross the midline of the embryo to form synapses with cells on the other side. This organization is similar to that of the ascending fibres which carry pain information from the spinal cord in mammals (the fibres cross the midline within the brain before reaching the higher order somatosensory areas).

It is, however, unclear whether or not the fruit fly's brain is involved in the rolling behaviour. The fruit fly larva's response to noxious stimuli may be more like a reflex: the sensory neurons that detect the stimulus transmit signals to the motor neurons which control the muscles, in the absence of any involvement by the brain.

However, rolling and similar behaviours involve complex and co-ordinated patterns of muscle activity throughout the body, and so may be under the control of central pattern generators in the brain. But even if the brain is involved, it is unlikely that fruit flies feel pain as we do, because human pain has emotional aspects which are processed in the association areas of the cerebral cortex.

Reference:

Hwang, R. Y., et al. (2007). Nociceptive Neurons Protect Drosophila Larvae from Parasitoid Wasps. Curr. Biol. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.029 [Abstract]

More like this

THE humble fruit fly (Drosophila melanogaster) has the ability to learn and remember, and to make predictions about the outcome of its behaviours on the basis of past experience. Compared to a human brain, that of the fruit fly  is relatively simple, containing approximately 250,000 cells. Even so…
[Introduction|Part 2|Part 3] The study by McKemy et al is of great significance, as it led to the identification and characterization of the first cold receptor. This study also suggests that TRP channels have a general role in thermosensation, as all the previously identified TRP channels are…
OF all the techniques used by neuroscientists, none has captured the imagination of the general public more than functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The technique, which is also referred to as functional neuroimaging and, more commonly, "brain scanning", enables us to peer into the human…
WHO could have guessed that a protein isolated from pond scum would transform the way researchers investigate the brain? The protein, called channelrhodopsin (ChR), is found in algae and other microbes, and is related to the molecule in human photoreceptors that captures light particles. Both…

Very interesting. Philosophers of mind are always using the toy example of "pain" = "c-fibers firing." Could you direct an interested philosopher to a good account of the current neurological understanding of pain?

By Physicalist (not verified) on 01 Dec 2007 #permalink

A neuron receptor that elicits a withdrawal response - yes I can understand that would be common in many co-evolved animals. But to call the result "pain", surely that requires connection to a more complex processing system? Yes it is good research and an excellent article, but I think that language may encourage the anti-science anti-experiment lobbies?

Actually, this work was done at Duke University in the laboratory of Dan Tracey, who is the senior and corresponding author of the paper. Karl Diesseroth's lab at Stanford developed the light-triggered activity modulator technique, but Dan is the expert on insect pain and led the team that performed these experiments.

I've been becoming more curious as I've been reading your articles about just how much consciousness insects really do have, and this post just deepens that curiosity. I sincerely do hope pain is merely a reflex in them in their vicious kingdom. Plus, I've also swatted quite a few, living here in the bug ridden American South.

By carolyn13 (not verified) on 02 Dec 2007 #permalink

Of course insects feel pain. Men base their entire system of torture on this fact. Boys begin their lives pulling the legs off of grasshoppers and flys. They grow up to blast them off of each other. What could be more obvious?

But even if the brain is involved, it is unlikely that fruit flies feel pain as we do, because human pain has emotional aspects which are processed in the association areas of the cerebral cortex.

I agree with carolyn13. I hope it is just a reflex. It seems from everything here that there is no conclusive evidence that a stimuli makes it all the way through the nervous system to the brain and registers as pain.
Dave Briggs :~)

"human pain has emotional aspects which are processed in the association areas of the cerebral cortex."

Well, then do dogs feel pain? Cats? Penned up cattle and hogs on the way to the slaughterhouse? Chickens?

Where is the cut-off point? Insects don't feel pain. Mammals do. But not all mammals feel pain the same way, because certain groups have um higher pain thresholds than others.

Sophistry.

By degustibus (not verified) on 06 Dec 2007 #permalink

All the animals you listed experience emotions, you know.